Abstract
Dystonia is a neurological disorder characterized by involuntary repetitive movements and abnormal postures. Animal models have played a pivotal role in studying the pathophysiology of dystonia. However, many genetic models, e.g., the (DYT1) mouse, lack an overt motor phenotype, despite significant underlying neuronal abnormalities within the striatum and other motor control regions. Because the striatum is implicated in action sequencing, it is possible that the behavioral defect arises as a disruption in the frequency and temporal ordering of behaviors, rather than execution, which cannot be captured using traditional behavioral assays, thus limiting drug discovery efforts. To address this challenge, we used MoSeq, an unsupervised behavioral segmentation framework, to compare the continuous free behavior of control mice and knockin mutant mice in response to the anti-dystonia drug trihexyphenidyl. Although minimal baseline differences in behavioral organization were detected, both genotypes exhibited robust and consistent shifts in behavioral space structure after treatment with trihexyphenidyl. Further, we demonstrate differences in the behavioral space structure of male vs. female mice after trihexyphenidyl challenge. The distinct behavioral signatures evoked by trihexyphenidyl and biological sex, a known risk factor for dystonia, suggest that the analysis of the temporal structure of continuous free behavior provides a sensitive and novel approach to the discovery of therapeutics for the treatment of dystonia.
Graphical Abstract

Introduction
Dystonia is a family of motor abnormalities characterized by involuntary muscle contractions causing sustained or transient twisting movements, abnormal postures, or repetitive motions. The etiology is heterogeneous, including genetic mutations and brain injuries, but most dystonias are idiopathic and are more common in women than in men [1–3]. This complex heterogeneity has led to the creation of various genetic, pharmacological, and lesion-induced animal models of dystonia [4, 5] which have, in turn, provided insight into the underlying neuronal dysfunction. Notably, TOR1A (DYT1) dystonia is often used as a model system for understanding both the defects underlying dystonia and the mechanism of action of trihexyphenidyl, a small molecule drug frequently used to treat symptoms of dystonia. TOR1A dystonia is caused by a gag deletion (E) in the TOR1A gene. Although it is a dominantly inherited disorder, only 30% of carriers express dystonia while most do not manifest overt motor dysfunction. Genetically engineered mouse models of TOR1A dystonia exhibit striatal dysfunction including aberrant cholinergic tone, reduced striatal dopamine transmission, and defects in corticostriatal synaptic plasticity [6–14], which are corrected by trihexyphenidyl (THP). Despite the neuronal dysfunction, these mice lack an overt behavioral phenotype and exhibit only subtle motor signs when challenged [6, 15], suggesting that the models are akin to non-manifesting carriers.
Despite recent advancements in behavioral tracking methods, objective and holistic assessments of continuous free behavior in these models are still lacking. Subsequently, it has remained hard to draw a link between pathology and exhibited behavior, assess the severity of a phenotype in a given model, or test the effectiveness of potential pharmacological treatments. Traditionally used behavioral assays such as reflex and motor coordination assays are often limited by sensitivity to procedural and environmental variables, challenges in reproducibility, and observer subjectivity. For example, in the hindlimb clasping test, several factors could affect the outcome of the reflex, including the vertical distance from the floor, handling technique, duration of suspension, etc. Moreover, the results of some of these assays can be challenging to interpret, as no clear specificity between the assay and a particular trait can be established. For instance, an increased number of failed rotarod trials cannot be attributed to a single factor or dystonic trait. Additionally, these models may display covert deviations in their behavioral repertoires that may not be easily detected with traditional assays.
Because the striatum plays a critical role in the temporal organization of ethologically-relevant action sequences [16–19], changes in striatal function caused by mutations, drugs, or insult may result in covert deviations from normal behavioral repertoires that are not easily detected using traditional assays, which typically focus on a single behavioral task. Given the known striatal dysfunction in mouse models of TOR1A dystonia, these mice may be able to execute spontaneous actions indistinguishably from control mice, however, the stochastic order with which they transition from one action to another or the frequency at which they express a certain action may differ. Likewise, THP, which is known to mediate striatal function, may elicit a behavioral signature. Such changes in behavior are not easily resolved via traditional behavioral assays because they are specifically designed to assess the performance of action execution. Thus, a comprehensive assay that assesses the overall continuous free mouse behavior while taking into consideration its temporal structure may be better suited to detecting deviations from typical behavior.
To determine if the (gag) mutation or THP alters the structure of spontaneous free behavior, we used MoSeq [20], an unsupervised behavioral tracking and segmentation framework grounded in the ethological observation that behavior is composed of organized probabilistic sequences of behaviors. MoSeq uncovers the collective space of sub-second behavioral motifs, or syllables, that constitute continuous free behavior in mice, and summarizes the statistics of how mice traverse that space. Further, it was previously demonstrated that MoSeq has sufficient resolution to detect underlying striatal dysfunction based on deviations in the sequencing of mouse behavior [16]. Here, we present an application of the MoSeq segmentation pipeline to 3D recordings of freely behaving control and mutant mice. While the behavioral space structure of mice was similar to mice, the behavioral space structure differed by sex and was significantly altered after treatment with THP, providing a robust behavioral signature that may be useful for the identification of therapeutics for the treatment of dystonia.
Materials and methods
Mice
Heterozygous knockin mice carrying the Tor1a(gag) mutation [21] and control littermates inbred on C57BL/6J were bred at Emory University. Mice were housed with a 12-hour light/dark cycle. Food and water were provided ad libitum. Mice were genotyped using PCR (forward primer 5′-GCTATGGAAGCTCTAGTTGG-3′; reverse primer 5′-CAGCCAGGGCTAAACAGAG-3′). All experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Emory University and followed guidelines set forth in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Experimental design
Seventeen mice (8–10 weeks of age) were tested: 10 mutant (5 males and 5 females) and 7 control littermates (3 males and 4 females). Mice were tested using a cross-over design with 1 week between test sessions. On the day prior to recording, mice were habituated in the open field arena for 30 min. On the following day, each mouse was injected subcutaneously with either saline or 20 mg/kg trihexyphenidyl hydrochloride [TCI America], a dose that normalizes extracellular dopamine in mutant mice [22]. Treatments were administered in pseudorandom order. Mice were then placed in the open field arena for 10 min before recording of the freely-moving behavior commenced (Figure 1A, top). Each recording session lasted 30–35 min and occurred between 9:30am and 3pm. Mice that received saline in the first test session received THP in the second session, and vice versa.
FIGURE 1

Illustration of the experimental apparatus and analysis pipeline. (A) Each mouse was habituated to the open field arena (bottom) for 30 min on the day prior to recording. On the following day, the mouse was injected, placed in the arena for 10 min before recording commenced. A camera directly above the arena recorded depth images and streamed them directly to disk. (B) Left: each row represents the extracted mouse from the recorded depth video from four time points throughout a rearing behavior both in the orientation as originally recorded (left column) and flipped to always face right (right column). The pseudocolor represents the height of the mouse. The dotted red arrows show the orientation of the mouse before and after the alignment and flipping procedure; middle: A sample trajectory in the top three PCs obtained by projecting the aligned, flipped mouse at every frame during the rearing behavior; right: the top 10 PCs are used as input data to a hierarchical autoregressive hidden Markov model (AR-HMM) whose hidden states represent the interpretable segmented behavioral syllables.
Data acquisition
Freely-moving mice were assessed in an open field that consisted of a 30x30x30 cm arena using a Helios 2+ time-of-flight depth sensor [Lucid Vision Labs, Canada] (Figure 1A, bottom) top-mounted 52 cm above the arena floor. Frames were acquired at 30 Hz and each frame was 640x480 pixels, where each pixel’s value specifies the distance from the sensor. The acquisition clock was set via a hardware trigger using an Arduino UNO board. Data acquisition was managed using custom Python scripts running on a Linux machine to which uncompressed frames were directly streamed. Each recording session totaled approximately 30GB.
Preprocessing & data preparation
All preprocessing was performed using custom Python scripts. For each recording, a rectangular region of interest that included only the arena boundaries was cropped to eliminate all other objects in the field of view. In each recording frame, all out-of-range pixel artifacts were replaced with “not-a-number” values to be ignored and facilitate background isolation at later analysis stages. Pixel values were scaled to millimeters as 16-bit integers, and the resulting raw file was converted to a lossless .avi file for use in downstream steps.
Analysis pipeline
MoSeq is an unsupervised machine learning framework that segments free behavior and parses out discrete, sub-second stereotyped behavioral motifs, or syllables. It relies on the observation that continuous free behavior can be modeled as the organization of stereotyped action modules into sequences via a probabilistic selection mechanism [20, 23]. The version of MoSeq used here utilizes mouse 3D pose dynamics measured by a top-mounted depth camera. The full MoSeq analysis pipeline is detailed in [20, 24] and its publicly available code repository [25]. Briefly, the background of each depth recording was calculated as the median of a subset of filtered frames sampled at 500-frame intervals throughout the recording. The background was then subtracted to highlight the mouse. An expectation-maximization (EM) tracking model was used to identify the mouse in the presence of noise, such as dust particles close to the sensor, as described in [16]. In short, the EM model assumes that the mouse in a given depth frame can be well-approximated by a 3D Gaussian. In each frame, the Gaussian’s parameters were first estimated using the fit from the previous frame and then progressively optimized through iterative EM. The model’s outputs, which are likelihood-weighted pixels, were subsequently used to determine the mouse’s center and orientation via an ellipse fit. This process extracts the mouse from the surrounding arena and enables kinematics calculations such as body-averaged 3D speed and height at each frame. The speed corresponds to the motion of the center of the ellipse while the height is calculated as the average height across all the pixels within the ellipse.
Next, in each frame, an 80x80 pixel box centered around the mouse was cropped and aligned along the major axis of the ellipse, i.e., the spinal axis, such that the mouse was aligned horizontally (Figure 1B, left). A random forest model was then used to automate the detection of the mouse orientation (nose facing left or right), and the cropped box was flipped accordingly to ensure that the mouse was always facing right by convention. Samples from the processed recordings were reviewed by a human observer to ensure there were no artifacts and that flips in orientation were minimal. This alignment process ensured an approximate pixel-body part mapping, thus allowing for meaningful dimensionality reduction. All aligned cropped frames across all recordings were used to learn a low-dimensional projection using Principal Component Analysis (PCA), and each recording’s cropped aligned frames were then projected onto the top 10 principal components forming a 10-dimensional time series summarizing the 3D pose trajectory over the course of the recording (Figure 1B, middle). The resulting PCA-projected pose summaries, which explained 85% of the variance, were used to fit a generative hierarchical autoregressive (AR) hidden Markov model whose hidden states represent the discrete identity of the behavioral syllable expressed, where each of these states is further represented via a separate AR process that describes the stereotyped continuous pose dynamics in Principal Component (PC) space. Model training was performed according to the MoSeq published workflows and code repository, as noted above.
Environmental variables and noise
Reflections were observed on two walls of the arena when mice reared on them. Therefore, preprocessing included an arena mask to minimize the reflections. To minimize noise in the depth images before projecting into the PC space, the right-aligned frames were smoothed in space using a 2D Gaussian filter with standard deviations pixels, and in time using a median filter of size 5 frames (i.e. 167 ms).
Quantitative discovered syllable validation
To validate that the states identified by MoSeq are coherent, distinct, and appropriately describe the dataset, we used a cross-likelihood validation metric as described in [20]. Briefly, cross-likelihood estimates how well the AR parameters associated with each hidden state can describe observations assigned to every other state. Values near or above 1 would indicate that the AR parameters of a state describe the observations associated with another state, while negative values indicate failure of the AR parameters of one state at describing the observations associated with another.
Statistical testing
After establishing that speed and distance values are normally distributed (using quantile-quantile plots), t-tests and ANOVAs with treatment as the repeated measure were performed using GraphPad Prism (version 10.4.1). We calculated bigram-normalized transition matrices, which represent the probability of a syllable occurring given the identity of the previous syllable, as the frequency of the syllable pair divided by the total occurrences of the first syllable. To compare these bigram-normalized transition matrices across groups, we used the Jensen-Shannon Divergence (JSD) metric [26] and assessed significance using permutation testing. For each pair of experimental groups, we computed the pairwise JSD between all session combinations across groups. The resulting JSD values were then averaged to obtain a mean observed JSD, representing the similarity between the two groups. Next, session group labels were randomly shuffled 10,000 times, recalculating the mean permuted JSD for each iteration. The p-value was determined as the fraction of permutations in which the permuted JSD was greater than or equal to the observed JSD.
Results
Aggregate kinematic summaries between genotypes
We first compared the gross kinematics, namely, speed and height, between control and mutant mice. By computing a probability distribution over all speed and height values exhibited by each mouse, we built an archetypal behavioral profile for each experimental group. This aggregate-level behavioral profile served as a preliminary screen to examine differences in the overall movement patterns between experimental groups and across single sessions. We found that saline-treated control and mutant mice displayed similar distributions of speeds and heights (Figures 2A,B, left column), suggesting that mutant mice exhibit an aggregate kinematic profile similar to control mice, in line with previous findings [6, 27]. Specifically, the total distance traveled in 30 min did not differ significantly between saline-treated control and mutant mice (57.74 4.10 and 49.62 3.71 m, respectively, , ). Likewise, the average speed did not show significant differences between the saline-treated control and mutant mice ( and mm/sec, respectively, , ). This approach provides a broad overview of behavioral trends, but it does not offer insights into the sequential organization of behavior. That is, the patterns of behavioral syllables that produce continuous free behavior may differ across groups that might otherwise share similar gross kinematics features.
FIGURE 2

Single-session aggregate kinematics summaries compared across pairs of experimental groups. (A) Top: The log-transformed probability distribution of the exhibited height. Bottom: The cumulative distribution function of the exhibited height. Each column represents a comparison across a pair of experimental groups. Cool and warm palettes represent control (Con; n = 7) and mutant (Mut; n = 10) genotypes, respectively. Light and dark lines represent saline and THP treatments, respectively. (B) Same as A but representing the distributions over exhibited speeds. The log-transformation was used to facilitate visualizing the heavy-tailed distributions. (C) Total distance traveled in 30 min by each experimental group. (D) Representative single-session occupancy heatmaps of saline-treated (top) and THP-treated (bottom) mice; left column represents control mice (Con), right column represents mutant mice (Mut).
Syllable expression frequency between genotypes
To resolve potential differences in the sequential and probabilistic structure of behavior between the two genotypes, we used MoSeq to uncover the sub-second behavioral syllables in our dataset to assess 1) how often control and mutant mice use each syllable and 2) the likelihood of transitioning from each syllable to every other syllable in each experimental group. MoSeq identified 44 coherent and stereotyped motifs, or syllables, that describe the behaviors in the dataset (Figure 3 and Supplementary Video S1). The syllables were visually inspected and further validated using a cross-likelihood estimate (Supplementary Figure S3) which showed effective separation of the syllables. At baseline, control and mutant mice exhibited similar syllable usage profiles (Figure 4A, top left).
FIGURE 3

MoSeq identifies coherent stereotyped behavioral syllables. Montages of “crowd” movies depicting four behavioral modules, or syllables. A “crowd movie” is created by overlaying movies from individual mice exhibiting the same behavioral syllable. By superimposing multiple individual movies into one, the stereotyped nature of each behavioral syllable is highlighted. Here we show a 5-frame montage of “crowd” movies corresponding to four behavioral syllables. Each row represents a different syllable and columns represent progression in time at ms increments with a discontinuity after 200 ms. The active expression of the syllable is marked by a red dot placed over each “mouse instance”. Note that frames before the dot onset and after the dot offset can represent different behavioral syllables.
FIGURE 4

Syllable usage across genotype and treatment. (A) Average syllable usage profiles of experimental group pairs. The syllables are sorted in descending order based on usage by the control group indicated on the x-axis in each panel to highlight trend differences. Top row shows comparison across genotypes; bottom row shows comparison across treatments; shaded area represents the SEM. (B) Two-dimensional projection of single sessions’ empirically-obtained syllable usage vectors on the top two principal components; blue and red colors represent control (Con) and mutant (Mut) genotypes, respectively; squares and circles represent males and females, respectively.
Syllable sequencing between genotypes
With access to the discrete behavioral syllables that describe our dataset and with a syllable identity assigned to each time point, it was possible to examine the sequential structure of mouse behavior across groups by computing a conditional probability distribution that quantifies the likelihood of transitioning from syllable A to any other syllable B. This distribution is captured by the bigram-normalized, empirically-observed transition matrix of a single session. To obtain a group-level representation, we averaged the observed transition matrices across mice within an experimental group. The resulting distribution was visualized as a transition graph where each node corresponds to a syllable and edges between nodes represent the transition probabilities (Figure 5A). To visually compare transition dynamics of a pair of groups, the corresponding transition graphs were subtracted to obtain a difference graph that highlights the differences in transition probabilities (Figure 5B). Visual inspection of the difference graphs suggested minimal differences between control and mutant mice. Further, quantitative assessment of the “distance” between transition probability distributions across groups, using the Jensen-Shannon divergence, showed no significant differences in transition dynamics between the two genotypes (Figure 5A).
FIGURE 5

Differences in the temporal organization of syllable expression across experimental groups. (A) Transition graphs representing averaged transition dynamics in control (Con) and mutant (Mut) mice in response to saline or THP challenge. Each node on the circumference represents a syllable and edge thickness represents the corresponding bigram-normalized transition likelihood. Statistical testing was performed using a permutation test with a JSD statistic, brackets indicate pairs of groups compared. (B) Difference transition graphs obtained by subtracting a pair of group-averaged transition graphs to highlight differences; blue and red edges represent upregulation and downregulation of a particular transition, respectively.
Behavioral space structure after THP challenge
After comparing the behavioral spaces of control and mutant mice, we then compared the effects of THP on behavioral composition. Both THP-treated control and mutant mice displayed vastly different speed and height distributions compared to saline treatment (Figures 2A,B, middle columns), demonstrating that the effect of THP can be resolved at this statistical summary level. Consistent with prior studies [28–30], THP treatment induced a significant increase in locomotor activity compared to saline treatment (Figures 2C, D) in both control and mutant (2-way repeated measures ANOVA, main effect of treatment, , ; main effect of genotype, , ). Moreover, we found that THP augmented fast behaviors in both control and mutant mice, as reflected in the initial slow rise of the speed cumulative distribution functions (CDFs) (Figure 2B, middle columns) and the average speed (Supplementary Figures S1C,D) in control and mutant mice(2-way repeated measures ANOVA, main effect of treatment, , ; main effect of genotype, , ). Additionally, THP-treated control and mutant mice displayed similar kinematics distributions (Figures 2A,B, right column), suggesting that both genotypes exhibit comparable behavioral responses to THP at this aggregate kinematics level.
THP administration resulted in the reorganization of syllable usage profiles in control and mutant mice (Figure 4A, top right). This THP-induced restructuring was robust (Figure 4A, bottom row), reflecting a substantial reorganization of behavioral output in response to THP in both control and mutant mice. Despite these pronounced changes, MoSeq revealed minimal differences in syllable frequency between control and mutant mice in response to THP. Indeed, the transition graphs illustrate the THP-induced change in the behavior of both genotypes and also demonstrate the similarity of the behavior elicited by THP in both genotypes. Syllable usage profiles visualized in PC space (Figure 4B) similarly revealed distinct clusters between saline treatment and THP treatment while the genotypes overlapped within each cluster. Surprisingly, we also observed a separation between males and females (with the exception of one THP-treated male) in PC space (described below and in Figure 6). To ensure that the treatment effect was not dominating the top PCs and diluting the genotype effect, we performed Linear Discriminant Analysis (LDA) with sex, treatment, and genotype separately to maximize class separability. We observed substantial overlap between the two genotypes across sex and treatments (Supplementary Figure S2), but notably, saline-treated males showed the largest separability compared to other groups. Additionally, quantitative assessment of the transition probability distributions showed no significant differences in transition dynamics between the genotypes regardless of treatment (saline or THP), whereas substantial differences were observed between saline baseline and THP challenge (Figure 5A) in both genotypes. In contrast to the PC analysis, difference transition graphs showed subtle differences in transition dynamics between the two genotypes in the saline baseline condition, but less so after THP administration (Figure 5B).
FIGURE 6

Syllable usage across sex within genotype-treatment combinations. Each panel corresponds to the average syllable usage profiles of a genotype-treatment combination specified in the x-axis label in control (Con) and mutant (Mut) mice. Syllables are sorted in descending order based on usage by male mice in that group. Cyan and magenta represent males and females, respectively. Shaded area represents the SEM.
Sex differences
The separation between males and females in PC space suggested underlying sex differences. Therefore, we compared distance traveled and kinematics between sexes in all experimental conditions. Females covered significantly more distance than males and the speed distributions suggested that females exhibited faster speeds than males after both saline and THP treatments (Supplementary Figures S1A,B). Further, the THP-induced increase in speed appeared more pronounced in females than in males, suggesting that the effect of THP is potentiated in females.
Discussion
We used a state-of-the-art method to segment continuous free behavior of control and mutant mice in an objective and unsupervised manner. Both genotypes displayed similar speed and height distributions and appeared to share a similar behavioral repertoire although subtle differences that did not reach significance were detected. While males and females exhibited unique behavioral landscapes, obvious differences between the genotypes within sex were not observed. In contrast, THP, an anticholinergic drug used to alleviate dystonia, significantly shifted the baseline behavioral landscape, suggesting a novel approach for the identification of antidystonic drugs.
The similarity in the temporal sequencing of syllables between mutant and control mice is remarkable given the deficit in dopamine release in the mutant mouse striatum [8, 22], which has been shown to play a critical role in probabilistic action sequencing [16–19]. While striatal lesions in adult mice result in overt behavioral changes, the Tor1a gene defect affects neuronal dysfunction throughout development [31], which may trigger a compensatory mechanism that stabilizes motor function and sequencing despite the 50% reduction in dopamine release in mutant mice. This mechanism could be a layer of homeostatic plasticity at the level of spiny projection neurons or a larger, network-level compensation involving the cortex.
Our approach was extremely sensitive for capturing the behavioral signature of the antidystonic drug THP, validating the sensitivity of the assay to behavioral change. In addition to a pronounced locomotion-enhancing effect, consistent with previous findings in rodents [28–30], we found that THP augmented fast behaviors and reorganized syllable usage. This robust behavioral response was observed in control and mutant mice, indicating that both genotypes share a common behavioral response to THP. One potential mechanism underlying this increase in locomotion activity and speed could be an increase in dopamine release in response to THP. This is supported by previous work demonstrating that THP increases dopamine release both in control and mutant knockin mice [14].
The mechanisms underlying the antidystonic effects of THP are unknown. However, it is known that THP rescues both the abnormal striatal dopamine release and the abnormal corticostriatal plasticity in mouse models of TOR1A dystonia. THP is a nonselective muscarinic receptor antagonist that binds with low nanomolar affinity to all five (M1-M5) muscarinic receptor subtypes [32, 33]. All muscarinic receptor subtypes are expressed in the striatum [34–39]. M1 and M4 mAChRs mediate corticostriatal plasticity [36, 37, 40, 41] and striatal dopamine release [42–45]. M2 and M3 mAChRs are located on corticostriatal terminals and mediate glutamatergic signaling [34, 35, 38]. M5 mAChR on dopaminergic terminals mediate striatal dopamine release [39, 46]. Thus, while it has been challenging to pinpoint the mechanism of action of THP, it is possible that THP exerts its effects through M1 and/or M4 muscarinic receptors as blocking either of these receptors rescues both the deficit in dopamine release and the impaired plasticity in models of TOR1A dystonia [12, 47–49]. However, it is important to note that for the behavioral results presented here, it is difficult to ascribe a specific underlying mechanism or neuronal target to explain the effects of peripherally administered THP, considering that muscarinic receptors are expressed peripherally and throughout the entire brain.
In addition to the effects of THP, there was a significant difference in the behavioral space between males and females. Biological sex is a known risk factor for most idiopathic focal dystonias, including blepharospasm, oromandibular dystonia, cervical dystonia, and spasmodic dysphonia [50–55]. For inherited generalized dystonias, male-predominance obviously occurs in X-linked disorders, such as X-linked dystonia-parkinsonism. However, in the largest study published to date, male-bias was not observed in dystonias with autosomal inheritance, including TOR1A and THAP1 dystonias [56]. In contrast, female bias occurs in some dystonias with autosomal inheritance including DOPA-responsive dystonia (DRD), which is caused by mutations in genes critical for dopamine synthesis, and dystonia caused by pathogenic variants of GNAL, which encodes the G-protein coupled to striatal D1 dopamine receptors [56, 57]. Notably, both of these female-predominant inherited dystonias are caused by pathogenic variants that disrupt dopamine neurotransmission. Despite these well-recognized sex differences, biological sex is rarely considered when developing therapeutics for dystonia, in part, because behavioral differences between the sexes are often subtle or undetectable. MoSeq provides a robust tool for assessing sex-biased therapeutics.
The version of MoSeq we used relies on a low-dimensional projection of aligned sequential depth images of mice as input to the autoregressive hidden Markov model (AR-HMM). Subsequently, variation in size of the mice in the dataset may have an impact on the behavioral segmentation performance of the trained model if these variations are considerable. One potential failure mode of the model in presence of such variations could be segmenting a stereotyped behavior into multiple disjoint syllables based on inherent size rather than the underlying pose dynamics. Since female mice are generally smaller in size than males, this might limit analyses regarding sex differences using syllable usage and transition statistics. However, methods to mitigate such limitations exist, such as performing a size normalization step to scale all mice into a common reference size before training the model, or using keypoint-MoSeq [58], which relies on trajectories of tracked keypoints as input instead of depth pose dynamics thus overcoming size-related limitations.
There are few drugs that are effective for the treatment of dystonia and there is currently no “gold standard” therapeutic. THP is frequently used to treat dystonia and is the only small molecule drug that has been proven effective in a double-blind placebo-controlled study in people living with dystonia [59]. However, the dose-limiting side-effects associated with this non-selective muscarinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist, including constipation, dry mouth, and urinary retention, curtail its chronic use in humans, leaving patients with few options. New therapeutics have been slow to reach patients, in part, because preclinical testing in animal models generally involves labor-intensive ex vivo physiologic or behavioral assays that require highly specialized technical expertise. Here, we have demonstrated that THP elicits a clear and reproducible behavioral signature in freely moving mice using a streamlined assay. It is not yet clear if the THP-induced behavioral landscape is predictive of antidystonic drug effects in humans because the antidystonia pharmacopeia is so limited. However, identification of the fundamental behavioral signature of antidystonic drugs in the context of biological sex using ethological behavior as a predictor, as described here, has the potential to accelerate drug discovery in much the same way that the Porsolt swim test facilitated the discovery of antidepressant drugs.
Statements
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal study was approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Emory University. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: AA and EH; Investigation: AA and YD; Coding and Formal Analysis: AA, JM, and EH; Visualization: AA, HJ, and EH; Writing – Original Draft: AA and EH; Writing – Review and Editing: AA, YD, JM, HJ, CP, and EH; Funding Acquisition: EH. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by National Institutes of Health grants R01 NS124764. JM is supported by a Career Award at the Scientific Interface from the Burroughs Wellcome Fund, the David and Lucille Packard Foundation, the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation, and the McCamish Foundation.
Conflict of interest
CP is a research scientist at Meta (Reality Labs).
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontierspartnerships.org/articles/10.3389/dyst.2025.15034/full#supplementary-material
SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE S1Kinematic summaries compared across sex and treatment. (A) Top: The log-transformed probability distribution of the exhibited speed. Bottom: The cumulative distribution function (CDF) of the exhibited speed. The left column shows all saline-treated mice (mutant and control). The right column shows all THP-treated mice (mutant and control). Cyan and magenta represent male and female mice, respectively. The speed distributions suggest that females exhibited generally faster speeds than males after both saline and THP challenge as reflected by the slower initial rise in the speed CDFs of female mice (B) Similar to A, showing the distribution of height values exhibited in each session. (C) Session-averaged speed in control (Con) and mutant (Mut) mice in response to saline or THP challenge. A 3-way ANOVA (genotype sex treatment with treatment as the repeated measure) reveals a significant sex treatment interaction effect (, ) with post-hoc t tests demonstrating that the difference in average speed was not significant across sex in saline-treated mice but significant in THP-treated mice , suggesting that the effect of THP is potentiated in females. (D) Similar to C but with the genotypes collapsed to show sex × treatment interaction. Asterisks indicate statistical significance . (E) The total distance traveled in control (Con) and mutant (Mut) mice in response to saline or THP challenge. A 3-way repeated measures ANOVA (genotype sex treatment with treatment as the repeated measure) demonstrated significant main effects of sex (, ) whereby females covered a larger distance than males, and treatment (, ), but no effect of genotype (, ). Interaction effects of genotype x sex, genotype x treatment, sex x treatment, or three-way interactions were not observed, (, , , , respectively), although some suggest a trend.
SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE S2Kernel density estimates of syllable usage profiles LDA projection scores. To ensure that THP treatment or sex differences were not diluting a genotype effect in the top PC dimensions, we performed LDA to reduce syllable usage vectors to a single discriminant axis where each sex-treatment combination was projected separately to emphasize any separation of genotypes. Kernel density estimation (KDE) was then applied to smooth the distribution of these projection scores to enable a clear visualization of density patterns and highlighting separation tendencies among the genotypes in each group.
SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE S3Pairwise syllable cross-likelihood analysis. A heatmap showing the cross-likelihood estimate between each pair of identified syllables. The cross-likelihood estimate represents the likelihood that a given occurrence of a syllable is well-modeled by another syllable, thus serving as a validation test of separation of syllables. Values near or above 1 would indicate that one syllable can effectively model another, while values well below 1 would indicate the contrary. Cross-likelihood estimates were calculated as described in Wiltschko et al. [20]. Units are in nats.
SUPPLEMENTARY VIDEO S1MoSeq identified coherent, stereotyped behavioral modules. “Crowd movies” showing six distinct behavioral syllables automatically identified by MoSeq along with a description provided by a human observer. “Crowd movies” are obtained by overlaying multiple occurrences of a specific syllable detected by the model from multiple recordings onto one arena space to highlight the stereotypy of each behavioral syllable. The active expression of the syllable is marked by a dot placed over each “mouse instance”. Note that frames before the dot onset and after the dot offset can represent different behavioral syllables. All instances are time-locked such that the active expression of the syllable occurs simultaneously in all mice to illustrate the stereotyped three-dimensional pose dynamics.
References
1.
Turcano P Savica R . Sex differences in movement disorders. Handbook Clin Neurol (2020) 175:275–82. 10.1016/B978-0-444-64123-6.00019-9
2.
Meoni S Macerollo A Moro E . Sex differences in movement disorders. Nat Rev Neurol (2020) 16:84–96. 10.1038/s41582-019-0294-x
3.
Rafee S O’Riordan S Reilly R Hutchinson M . We must talk about sex and focal dystonia. Movement Disord (2021) 36:604–8. 10.1002/mds.28454
4.
Wilson BK Hess EJ . Animal models for dystonia. Movement Disord (2013) 28:982–9. 10.1002/mds.25526
5.
Meringolo M Tassone A Imbriani P Ponterio G Pisani A . Dystonia: are animal models relevant in therapeutics?Revue Neurologique Int SFN/SOFMA Meet (2018) 174:608–14. 10.1016/j.neurol.2018.07.003
6.
Song CH Fan X Exeter CJ Hess EJ Jinnah HA . Functional analysis of dopaminergic systems in a DYT1 knock-in mouse model of dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2012) 48:66–78. 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.05.009
7.
Balcioglu A Kim MO Sharma N Cha JH Breakefield XO Standaert DG . Dopamine release is impaired in a mouse model of DYT1 dystonia. J Neurochem (2007) 102:783–8. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04590.x
8.
Page ME Bao L Andre P Pelta-Heller J Sluzas E Gonzalez-Alegre P et al Cell-autonomous alteration of dopaminergic transmission by wild type and mutant (DeltaE) TorsinA in transgenic mice. Neurobiol Dis (2010) 39:318–26. 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.04.016
9.
Martella G Maltese M Nisticò R Schirinzi T Madeo G Sciamanna G et al Regional specificity of synaptic plasticity deficits in a knock-in mouse model of DYT1 dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2014) 65:124–32. 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.01.016
10.
Pisani A Martella G Tscherter A Bonsi P Sharma N Bernardi G et al Altered responses to dopaminergic D2 receptor activation and N-type calcium currents in striatal cholinergic interneurons in a mouse model of DYT1 dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2006) 24:318–25. 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.07.006
11.
Grundmann K Glöckle N Martella G Sciamanna G Hauser TK Yu L et al Generation of a novel rodent model for DYT1 dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2012) 47:61–74. 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.03.024
12.
Martella G Tassone A Sciamanna G Platania P Cuomo D Viscomi MT et al Impairment of bidirectional synaptic plasticity in the striatum of a mouse model of DYT1 dystonia: role of endogenous acetylcholine. Brain (2009) 132:2336–49. 10.1093/brain/awp194
13.
Sciamanna G Tassone A Mandolesi G Puglisi F Ponterio G Martella G et al Cholinergic dysfunction alters synaptic integration between thalamostriatal and corticostriatal inputs in DYT1 dystonia. en J Neurosci (2012) 32:11991–2004. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-12.2012
14.
Downs AM Fan X Kadakia RF Donsante Y Jinnah HA Hess EJ . Cell-intrinsic effects of TorsinA(ΔE) disrupt dopamine release in a mouse model of TOR1A dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2021) 155:105369. 10.1016/j.nbd.2021.105369
15.
Dang MT Yokoi F McNaught KSP Jengelley TA Jackson T Li J et al Generation and characterization of Dyt1 DeltaGAG knock-in mouse as a model for early-onset dystonia. Exp Neurol (2005) 196:452–63. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.08.025
16.
Markowitz JE Gillis WF Beron CC Neufeld SQ Robertson K Bhagat ND et al The striatum organizes 3D behavior via moment-to-moment action selection. English Cell (2018) 174:44–58.e17. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.04.019
17.
Aldridge JW Berridge KC . Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence. J Neurosci (1998) 18:2777–87. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02777.1998
18.
Minkowicz S Mathews MA Mou FH Yoon H Freda SN Cui ES et al Striatal ensemble activity in an innate naturalistic behavior. eLife (2023) 12. 10.7554/eLife.87042.1
19.
Jin X Tecuapetla F Costa RM . Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nature Neurosci. (2014) 17 (3), 423–30. 10.1038/nn.3632
20.
Wiltschko AB Johnson MJ Iurilli G Peterson RE Katon JM Pashkovski SL et al Mapping sub-second structure in mouse behavior. Neuron (2015) 88:1121–35. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.031
21.
Goodchild RE Kim CE Dauer WT . Loss of the dystonia-associated protein torsinA selectively disrupts the neuronal nuclear envelope. Neuron (2005) 48:923–32. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.010
22.
Downs AM Fan X Donsante C Jinnah HA Hess EJ . Trihexyphenidyl rescues the deficit in dopamine neurotransmission in a mouse model of DYT1 dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2019) 125:115–22. 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.01.012
23.
Tinbergen N . The Study of Instinct. en. Google-Books-ID: a1sPAAAAMAAJ. Clarendon Press (1951).
24.
Wiltschko AB Tsukahara T Zeine A Anyoha R Gillis WF Markowitz JE et al Revealing the structure of pharmacobehavioral space through motion sequencing. Nat Neurosci (2020) 23:1433–43. 10.1038/s41593-020-00706-3
25.
Datta Lab. MoSeq2 app (2021). GitHub. Available online at: https://github.com/dattalab/moseq2-app. Commit a09afb9 (Accessed February 26, 2025).
26.
Lin J . Divergence measures based on the Shannon entropy. IEEE Trans Inf Theor (1991) 37:145–51. 10.1109/18.61115
27.
Sharma N Baxter MG Petravicz J Bragg DC Schienda A Standaert DG et al Impaired motor learning in mice expressing TorsinA with the DYT1 dystonia mutation. en J Neurosci (2005) 25:5351–5. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0855-05.2005
28.
Shimosato K Watanabe S Kitayama S . Differential effects of trihexyphenidyl on place preference conditioning and locomotor stimulant activity of cocaine and methamphetamine. en Naunyn-schmiedeberg’s Arch Pharmacol (2001) 364:74–80. 10.1007/s002100100433
29.
Tanda G Ebbs AL Kopajtic TA Elias LM Campbell BL Newman AH et al Effects of muscarinic M1 receptor blockade on cocaine-induced elevations of brain dopamine levels and locomotor behavior in rats. The J Pharmacol Exp Ther (2007) 321:334–44. 10.1124/jpet.106.118067
30.
Sipos ML Burchnell V Galbicka G . Dose-response curves and time-course effects of selected anticholinergics on locomotor activity in rats. Psychopharmacology (1999) 147:250–6. 10.1007/s002130051164
31.
Li J Levin DS Kim AJ Pappas SS Dauer WT . TorsinA restoration in a mouse model identifies a critical therapeutic window for DYT1 dystonia. The J Clin Invest (2021) 131:e139606. 10.1172/JCI139606
32.
Bolden C Cusack B Richelson E . Antagonism by antimuscarinic and neuroleptic compounds at the five cloned human muscarinic cholinergic receptors expressed in Chinese hamster ovary cells. The J Pharmacol Exp Ther (1992) 260:576–80. 10.1016/s0022-3565(25)11335-9
33.
Dörje F Wess J Lambrecht G Tacke R Mutschler E Brann MR . Antagonist binding profiles of five cloned human muscarinic receptor subtypes. The J Pharmacol Exp Ther (1990) 256(2):727–33. 10.1016/S0022-3565(25)23208-6
34.
Alcantara AA Mrzljak L Jakab RL Levey AI Hersch SM Goldman-Rakic PS . Muscarinic m1 and m2 receptor proteins in local circuit and projection neurons of the primate striatum: anatomical evidence for cholinergic modulation of glutamatergic prefronto-striatal pathways. The J Comp Neurol (2001) 434:445–60. 10.1002/cne.1186
35.
Buckley NJ Bonner TI Brann MR . Localization of a family of muscarinic receptor mRNAs in rat brain. The J Neurosci The Official J Soc Neurosci (1988) 8:4646–52. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-12-04646.1988
36.
Harrison MB Tissot M Wiley RG . Expression of m1 and m4 muscarinic receptor mRNA in the striatum following a selective lesion of striatonigral neurons. Brain Res (1996) 734(1):323–6. 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00785-8
37.
Hernández-Flores T Hernández-González O Pérez-Ramírez MB Lara-González E Arias-García MA Duhne M et al Modulation of direct pathway striatal projection neurons by muscarinic M4-type receptors. Neuropharmacology (2015) 89:232–44. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.09.028
38.
Levey AI Kitt CA Simonds WF Price DL Brann MR . Identification and localization of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor proteins in brain with subtype-specific antibodies. en J Neurosci (1991) 11:3218–26. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-10-03218.1991
39.
Yamada M Basile AS Fedorova I Zhang W Duttaroy A Cui Y et al Novel insights into M5 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor function by the use of gene targeting technology. Signal Transduction Drug Effects (2003) 74:345–53. 10.1016/j.lfs.2003.09.022
40.
Nair AG Castro LRV El Khoury M Gorgievski V Giros B Tzavara ET et al The high efficacy of muscarinic M4 receptor in D1 medium spiny neurons reverses striatal hyperdopaminergia. Neuropharmacology (2019) 146:74–83. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.11.029
41.
Nair AG Gutierrez-Arenas O Eriksson O Vincent P Kotaleski JH . Sensing positive versus negative reward signals through adenylyl cyclase-coupled GPCRs in direct and indirect pathway striatal medium spiny neurons. J Neurosci (2015) 35:14017–30. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0730-15.2015
42.
Foster DJ Wilson JM Remke DH Mahmood MS Uddin MJ Wess J et al Antipsychotic-like effects of M4 positive allosteric modulators are mediated by CB2 receptor-dependent inhibition of dopamine release. Eng Neuron (2016) 91:1244–52. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.017
43.
Gerber DJ Sotnikova TD Gainetdinov RR Huang SY Caron MG Tonegawa S . Hyperactivity, elevated dopaminergic transmission, and response to amphetamine in M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2001) 98:15312–7. 10.1073/pnas.261583798
44.
Pancani T Bolarinwa C Smith Y Lindsley CW Conn PJ Xiang Z . M4 mAChR-mediated modulation of glutamatergic transmission at corticostriatal synapses. ACS Chem Neurosci (2014) 5:318–24. 10.1021/cn500003z
45.
Threlfell S Clements MA Khodai T Pienaar IS Exley R Wess J et al Striatal muscarinic receptors promote activity dependence of dopamine transmission via distinct receptor subtypes on cholinergic interneurons in ventral versus dorsal striatum. The J Neurosci The Official J Soc Neurosci (2010) 30:3398–408. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5620-09.2010
46.
Foster DJ Gentry PR Lizardi-Ortiz JE Bridges TM Wood MR Niswender CM et al M5 receptor activation produces opposing physiological outcomes in dopamine neurons depending on the receptor’s location. The J Neurosci (2014) 34:3253–62. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4896-13.2014
47.
Dang MT Yokoi F Cheetham CC Lu J Vo V Lovinger DM et al An anticholinergic reverses motor control and corticostriatal LTD deficits in Dyt1 ΔGAG knock-in mice. Behav Brain Res (2012) 226:465–72. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.002
48.
Downs AM Donsante Y Jinnah HA Hess EJ . Blockade of M4 muscarinic receptors on striatal cholinergic interneurons normalizes striatal dopamine release in a mouse model of TOR1A dystonia. Neurobiol Dis (2022) 168:105699. 10.1016/j.nbd.2022.105699
49.
Maltese M Martella G Madeo G Fagiolo I Tassone A Ponterio G et al Anticholinergic drugs rescue synaptic plasticity in DYT1 dystonia: role of M1 muscarinic receptors. Movement Disord : official J Movement Disord Soc (2014) 29:1655–65. 10.1002/mds.26009
50.
Williams L McGovern E Kimmich O Molloy A Beiser I Butler JS et al Epidemiological, clinical and genetic aspects of adult onset isolated focal dystonia in Ireland. Eur J Neurol (2017) 24:73–81. 10.1111/ene.13133
51.
Hintze JM Ludlow CL Bansberg SF Adler CH Lott DG . Spasmodic dysphonia: a Review. Part 1: pathogenic factors. Otolaryngology–Head Neck Surg (2017) 157:551–7. 10.1177/0194599817728521
52.
Pandey S Sharma S . Meige’s syndrome: history, epidemiology, clinical features, pathogenesis and treatment. J Neurol Sci (2017) 372:162–70. 10.1016/j.jns.2016.11.053
53.
The Epidemiological Study of Dystonia in Europe (ESDE) Collaborative Group. A prevalence study of primary dystonia in eight European countries. J Neurol (2000) 247:787–92. 10.1007/s004150070094
54.
Marras C Van den Eeden SK Fross RD Benedict-Albers KS Klingman J Leimpeter AD et al Minimum incidence of primary cervical dystonia in a multiethnic health care population. Neurology (2007) 69:676–80. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000267425.51598.c9
55.
Kilic-Berkmen G Scorr LM McKay L Thayani M Donsante Y Perlmutter JS et al Sex differences in dystonia. Movement Disord Clin Pract (2024) 11:973–82. 10.1002/mdc3.14059
56.
Lange LM Junker J Loens S Baumann H Olschewski L Schaake S et al Genotype-phenotype relations for isolated dystonia genes: MDSGene systematic Review. Movement Disord (2021) 36:1086–103. 10.1002/mds.28485
57.
Wijemanne S Jankovic J . Dopa-responsive dystonia-clinical and genetic heterogeneity. Nat Rev Neurol (2015) 11:414–24. 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.86
58.
Weinreb C Pearl JE Lin S Osman MAM Zhang L Annapragada S et al Keypoint-MoSeq: parsing behavior by linking point tracking to pose dynamics. Nat Methods (2024) 21:1329–39. 10.1038/s41592-024-02318-2
59.
Burke RE Fahn S Marsden CD . Torsion dystonia. Neurology (1986) 36:160–0. 10.1212/WNL.36.2.160
Summary
Keywords
dystonia, behavioral segmentation, trihexyphenidyl, striatum, dopamine
Citation
Abdal Qader A, Donsante Y, Markowitz JE, Jinnah HA, Pandarinath C and Hess EJ (2025) Behavioral signature of trihexyphenidyl in the TOR1A (DYT1) knockin mouse model of dystonia. Dystonia 4:15034. doi: 10.3389/dyst.2025.15034
Received
04 June 2025
Accepted
28 July 2025
Published
15 August 2025
Volume
4 - 2025
Edited by
Daniela Popa, INSERM U1024 Institut de biologie de l’Ecole Normale Supérieure, France
Updates
Copyright
© 2025 Abdal Qader, Donsante, Markowitz, Jinnah, Pandarinath and Hess.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ellen J. Hess, ellen.hess@emory.edu
Disclaimer
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.